These findings raise the possibility that dopamine

These findings raise the possibility that dopamine

selleck inhibitor release might subserve multiple functions, conveying different signals to different parts of the brain in order to meet a variety of behavioral demands. Yet a clear delineation of what functions these disparate signals perform has been lacking. In this issue, Matsumoto and Takada (2013) set out to remedy this gap by studying the diversity of dopamine signaling across the midbrain during cognitive performance. To do this, they recorded single neurons from the ventral tegmental area (VTA) and substantia nigra pars compacta (SNc) in monkeys performing a visual search task for fluid reward. On most trials, monkeys were first shown a cue indicating whether a large or small reward would be delivered for a correct response. This cue was followed by a sample stimulus (a slanted line). The monkeys were then shown an array of slanted lines (two, four, or six items), selleck products among which they had to search for a match to the sample stimulus. Monkeys indicated a match by visually fixating the matching target. Previous work has shown that dopamine is necessary for maintaining working memory (Li and Mei, 1994, Sawaguchi and Goldman-Rakic, 1991, Sawaguchi and Goldman-Rakic, 1994, Watanabe et al.,

1997 and Williams and Goldman-Rakic, 1995), as well as for facilitating visual perception (Noudoost and Moore, 2011), and thus might be released in response to the display of the target cue. Yet, this should only be necessary when the information in the sample stimulus is needed for the upcoming search. To test this, the authors interleaved blocks of the match-to-sample task with blocks of a second visual search task. In this second task, a slanted line stimulus was again presented, but the search array consisted of unrelated shapes (triangles and squares). The monkey’s

task was then simply to locate the lone triangle, which “popped out” from the array. For this task, the initial stimulus was unnecessary, and no working memory was required. The results of Matsumoto and Takada’s experiment are summarized in Figure 1. As expected, dopamine neurons responded more strongly to Sodium butyrate the cue advertising a large reward than to the cue for a small reward (A). More importantly, cells responded much more strongly to the sample stimulus when it was needed for the upcoming search than when it was irrelevant, suggesting that dopamine release from midbrain neurons contributes to the working memory requirements of the match-to-sample task (B). In addition, dopamine cells fired more strongly to the onset of smaller, easier arrays than to larger, harder ones (C) and responded more strongly when monkeys found targets in large arrays than in small ones (D).

Although statistically significant in some cases, these defects w

Although statistically significant in some cases, these defects were less severe compared

to what we observed in the Pou3f4 or Epha4 mutants, probably owing to functional redundancy by other ephrins present or possibly a lack of complete ephrin-B2 elimination. Efnb2 cko cochleae showed a 6% increase in area consumed by axons and maximally a 4-fold increase in the number of axons that crossed between bundles ( Figures 7R and 7S). Because tamoxifen was given at a single dose at E9.5–E10.5, most defects were observed at the base and midmodiolar XAV-939 molecular weight regions of the cochlea, in accordance with previous studies with Ngn-CreERT2 ( Koundakjian et al., 2007). Importantly, the Efnb2 cko phenotype was similar to the fasciculation phenotypes observed in both the Pou3f4 PLX 4720 and Epha4 mutants lines, suggesting that ephrin-B2 may function as a ligand for EphA4 in this process. We next asked whether, in the developing otic mesenchyme, regulatory elements of Epha4 are a direct target of Pou3f4. Pou proteins are known to have a bipartite DNA binding system that includes a POU-specific domain,

as well as a homeobox domain ( Phillips and Luisi, 2000). Whereas the recognition sequences of several Pou proteins have been characterized, relatively little is known about mouse Pou3f4. However, one report has demonstrated that Pou3f4 preferentially recognizes the tandem homeobox sequence ATTATTA in the regulation of the dopamine receptor 1A gene D1A ( Okazawa et al., 1996). Therefore, we scanned the entire Epha4 genomic sequence and found four of these sites within introns in the first 70 kilobases (kb) and one approximately 8.5 kb upstream of exon 1 ( Figure 8A). We then performed chromatin immunoprecipitation (ChIP) using otic mesenchyme and a Pou3f4-specific IgY and assayed the resulting DNAs for these Epha4 regulatory regions using quantitative PCR ( Figure 8B). In these experiments β-actin, Neurogenin-1,

Idoxuridine and an Epha4 site that did not contain ATTATTA were used as negative controls; there was no significant association at these sites with the control or Pou3f4-specific IgY. However, all five of the putative Epha4 regulatory regions showed preferential association (with statistical significance) with the Pou3f4-specific IgY, with little to no association with the control IgY ( Figure 8B). These data suggest that Pou3f4 may directly regulate Epha4 in otic mesenchyme cells in order to initiate EphA4-mediated SGN fasciculation. The results presented in this study reveal a new and intriguing role for otic mesenchyme in the development of cochlear innervation. Moreover, the identification of additional auditory defects arising from the absence of Pou3f4 provides insights into the underlying basis for the deafness that occurs in both human and murine mutants.

We find that, similar to the results with cultured neurons, AAK1

We find that, similar to the results with cultured neurons, AAK1 siRNA increased proximal branching in vivo (Figures 6I and 6J). Next, we investigated Rabin8′s function on dendrite development and spine

maturation in hippocampal cultures. Immunostaining of endogeneous Rabin8 by anti-Rabin8 antibody showed that Rabin8 is enriched in the Golgi (colocalized with Golgi marker GM-130; Figure 7A), in agreement with the role of Rab8 in post-Golgi trafficking. We first examined its function by mutating the Rabin8 phosphorylation INCB018424 chemical structure site and expressing these mutants in dissociated hippocampal neurons. We made the Rabin8 phospho mutant, where S240 as well as T241, S242, and S243 were mutated to Alanine (Rabin8-AAAA), which cannot be phosphorylated (Figure 5F), or to Glutamate (Rabin8-EEEE) as a putative phosphomimetic mutant. We found that these Rabin8 mutants and Rabin8 siRNA (Figures S6A and S7A) did not affect dendrite branching (Figures

S6C–S6F), indicating that Rabin8 phosphorylation by NDR1 is likely not involved in limiting dendrite branching. The total dendrite length was reduced by Rabin8-AAAA but not Rabin8 siRNA (Figure S6F). Given that Rabin8 siRNA may not have sufficiently knocked down the Rabin8 level, these observations indicate that Rabin8 is involved in dendrite growth. Next, we found that the expression of Rabin8-AAAA but not Rabin8-EEEE resulted in increased selleck chemicals llc filopodia and atypical spines, and Rabin8 siRNA increased filopodia density (Figures 7B and 7C). An increase in filopodia was accompanied by a reduction in mushroom spine density by Rabin-AAAA, a trend Phosphoprotein phosphatase that was close to reaching significance (p = 0.07). These data indicate that Rabin8 phosphorylation by NDR1/2 contributes to spine development by reducing filopodia and increasing mushroom spines. Rabin8-AAAA and Rabin8 siRNA produce less pronounced defects on spines than does NDR1/2 loss of function, possibly

because other NDR1/2 substrates act in parallel to Rabin8 and contribute to spine morphogenesis. Alternatively, it is possible that these manipulations do not completely block Rabin8 function because of their incomplete knockdown or dominant negative effect. Given that Rabin-EEEE did not alter spine or dendrite development, this mutant construct may not be able to mimic phosphorylated Rabin8, a notion reinforced by our failed attempt to rescue NDR1siRNA + NDR2siRNA’s effect on spine development with Rabin8-EEEE (Figure S6B). Since Rabin8 is involved in spine maturation, we wanted to learn if it is present in spines with synapses. With immunostaining of postsynaptic marker PSD95 and endogenous Rabin8, we observe Rabin8 in the perinuclear region resembling Golgi and inside the proximal dendrites in neurons (Figure S6G). We cannot rule out the presence of Rabin8 in spines; however, the majority of Rabin8 is found in Golgi. (Figure S6G).

Very few labeled cell bodies were detected in the OB (Figure S1),

Very few labeled cell bodies were detected in the OB (Figure S1), and even these are likely newborn GCs migrating from the rostral migratory stream—such cells take more than 3 weeks to release GABA (Bardy et al., 2010) and should not contribute significantly toward direct release upon light stimulation. To confirm functional expression of ChR2 in AON neurons, we obtained whole-cell patch-clamp recordings

from AON neurons in acute slices from infected animals. Stimulation with blue light (whole field illumination, Topoisomerase inhibitor 5–10 mW/mm2) depolarized AON neurons sufficiently to evoke action potentials (Figure 1C). In fixed brain tissue, EYFP-positive axon terminals were clearly visible in the granule cell layer and the glomerular layer in both the ipsilateral and contralateral OB (Figure 1D).

The fluorescence intensity of EYFP per area unit was not uniform across the different layers of the OB, with greater intensities in the granule cell layer and the bottom part of the glomerular layer; fluorescence intensities were distinctly lower in the external plexiform layer, where most of the dendrodendritic synapses between MCs/TCs and GCs are located (Figure 1E). Contralateral projections to the glomerular layer had lower intensity than those in ipsilateral glomerular layer, even when normalized to their corresponding granule cell layer intensities (1.02 ± 0.09 versus 0.62 ± 0.13, n = 3, p < 0.05; Screening Library datasheet Figure 1F). These differences in average fluorescence intensities reflected the difference in density of fibers rather than expression levels of ChR2-EYFP, because the fluorescence intensity per area unit of single fibers in ipsilateral and contralateral OB were 1.00 ± 0.21 and 1.07 ± 0.28, respectively (n = 3 experiments, > 50 axons per experiment; errors are SD; Figures 1G and 1H), and not significantly different (p > 0.1). We examined synaptic responses of MCs to AON stimulation using whole cell recordings in acute OB slices that were made 2 to 4 weeks postinjection (Figure 2A). Excitatory and inhibitory synaptic currents were recorded in the voltage-clamp

mode at −70mV and 0mV, respectively, in response to a pair of 10 ms light pulses 100 ms apart. Although responses to pairs of stimuli are shown in the figures, all analysis mafosfamide reported below were done for the response to the first of the pair of stimuli. Light stimulation, unexpectedly, elicited excitatory as well as inhibitory synaptic currents in MCs, with inhibition being the dominant component (Figure 2B). All evoked currents were blocked by ionotropic glutamate receptor blockers (10 μM CNQX+ 100 μM APV; excitation blocked by 92.6% ± 4%, n = 3; inhibition blocked by 94.7% ± 3.1%, n = 4; p < 0.01; Figure 2C). Excitatory postsynaptic current (EPSC) amplitudes ranged from 5.8 to 29.1 pA and averaged 18.5 ± 6.6 pA (n = 15).

, 2008b), although there is some evidence for predictive signalin

, 2008b), although there is some evidence for predictive signaling even in the motor cortex (Flament and Hore, 1988). The argument for this hypothesis is as follows. During active movement, this population www.selleckchem.com/products/BKM-120.html may not be causally “driving” movement because it leads movement by only 50 ms instead of 100–150 ms, which is the typical “driving” delay seen in motor cortex during reaching movements (Ashe

and Georgopoulos, 1994, Moran and Schwartz, 1999 and Paninski et al., 2004). Instead, it could be predicting future movement direction 50 ms in advance of the actual movement (Figure 4C, right top panel, blue dashed line). The actual source of this predictive signaling could originate in some other cortical or subcortical area. During passive manipulation, one needs ABT-263 ic50 to assume that somatosensory feedback (i.e., tactile and proprioceptive input) can trigger covert motor commands much like the neural population described in the previous section that generated visually evoked covert motor commands. Somatosensory feedback would reach motor cortex with a delay of ∼50 ms (Figure 4C, right bottom panel, red curve). This input would trigger a covert

motor command leading the sensory feedback by ∼100 ms. If this population of neurons predicts the future sensory consequences of the covert motor command by 50 ms, then it would provide information preceding the sensory feedback by 50 ms (Figure 4C, right bottom panel, blue dashed line). Therefore, the predictive sensory lead in this population would offset the sensory delay in the periphery resulting in real-time tracking of movement. This hypothesis is further supported by the Ketanserin fact that the congruent subpopulation exhibited a 50% increase in peak directional information during passive movement as compared to the incongruent subpopulation indicating that the congruent subpopulation

is more faithfully capturing the detailed dynamics of movement. In the previous sections of this review, we have discussed literature demonstrating the richness and diversity in MI neural responses measured during the visual observation of familiar actions, passive movement of the limb, and voluntarily generated movements. This diversity is readily apparent in Figure 5, which shows the normalized binned firing rate as a function of time for each of the 87 neurons recorded during an experiment where monkeys generated active arm movements (blue region), observed playback of recorded movements with only visual (gold regions), proprioceptive (gray), or both types of feedback (red regions). Changes in the experimental condition were precisely correlated with substantial changes in the firing rate of individual neurons appearing as vertical striations in Figure 5. These heterogeneous responses are particularly interesting and potentially advantageous when placed in the context of a neuroprosthetic device or brain-machine interface (BMI).

However, the function of the premovement preparatory activity see

However, the function of the premovement preparatory activity seems not to be simply to rise to a point close to this boundary, as the rise-to-threshold hypothesis would suggest. Instead we note that the firing rates of some neurons fall (rather than rise) after the go cue, and—crucially—do so even if the firing of those same neurons had increased during the preparatory VE-822 research buy phase (Churchland et al., 2010a and Churchland and Shenoy, 2007b).

Thus, the path by which the system approaches this crossing point may be indirect. This observation was reflected in our RT predictions in two ways. First, the directions of the mean neural trajectory before and after the go cue ( p¯go−Δt′ and p¯go+Δt) differed, so that taking both into account improved RT predictions.

Second, three alternative schemes (see Experimental Procedures) that used the extent of rise or change in firing rates from their baseline values at the time of the go cue to predict RT did not perform as well; results from the best performing of the three are shown in Figure 5. Thus, we conclude that neural activity during movement preparation does not simply rise to, or directly approach, a movement-initiation threshold. The optimal subspace hypothesis suggests that for each possible desired movement goal there is a set of consistent preparatory network states, which all lead to movements C646 research buy that achieve that goal. The role of preparation is then to find one such state, and the computation necessary to do so is reflected in the dynamical evolution of the network state from its relatively uncontrolled pretask value to a point in the optimal subregion. Our results augment this view of preparation. There are many possible mechanisms by

which the preparatory activity may determine the activity associated with the execution of the movement, and thus the parameters of the movement itself. Our results suggest that the mechanism is, in fact, embodied in the dynamics of the network. It seems that the network activity evolves smoothly away from the optimal preparatory states when the movement is triggered. Thus, the optimality of the subregion may simply reflect the fact that all states within it form suitable initial conditions from which the dynamics of the network may evolve to generate the appropriate muscular control signals to generate Methisazone the corresponding movement. All such points may lead to movements that achieve the task goals adequately well. However, those states that happen to fall farther in the direction along which neural activity needs to evolve to generate the movement, and which reflect continued movement in that direction, allow the movement to begin sooner. Previous results that have provided evidence for the optimal subspace hypothesis remain consistent with this view. Preparatory activity must still reach the subregion of adequate initial conditions, leading to a fall in neural variance across trials during motor preparation (Churchland et al., 2006c).

, 2012) but underwent

additional experimental manipulatio

, 2012) but underwent

additional experimental manipulations for the http://www.selleckchem.com/products/fg-4592.html present work, and two additional rats were used exclusively for this study. The mean percentage of correct trials increased greatly over the course of learning, following a standard learning curve (Figure 1C). There was an initial phase of rapid improvement followed by a phase of slower learning, representing early (days 2–4) and late (days 8–11) learning. The percentage of correct trials increased significantly from early to late in learning (p < 0.001), demonstrating that rats were able to properly learn the task. Analyses of M1 firing rates further showed that rats were producing the desired ensemble rate modulations during task performance (Figure 1B). We first investigated the relationship between spiking activity and the LFP oscillations recorded during task engagement. We performed spike-triggered averaging of the LFP in late learning time locked to spikes occurring either in the same region or in the other region. If spiking activity was independent of LFP phase, then fluctuations would cancel and produce a flat average LFP. Instead, we observed clear mean LFP oscillations in both regions around action potentials from both regions; this oscillatory activity

had a strong component between 6–14 Hz (Figure 2A). This is consistent with past work showing that oscillations in this range are prominent in corticostriatal circuits when performing well-learned tasks (Berke et al., 2004), as well as work suggesting that M1 is predisposed to operate in this frequency range (Castro-Alamancos, 2013). We BLZ945 therefore filtered the raw LFP from 6–14 Hz and calculated the predominant phase at which spikes occurred. Again, we observed clear phase locking of spikes to the ongoing 6–14 Hz LFP in both regions (Figure 2B). Although the relationship between LFP and spiking is certainly complex and cells spike at several preferred LFP phases, there was nevertheless

a dominant phase preference across both regions. about Interestingly, both DS and M1 spikes occurred preferentially at the peak of the striatal 6–14 Hz LFP oscillation, suggesting that DS firing is maximal at the peak of the DS LFP. To further quantify these interactions and the ways they evolve during learning, we calculated coherence between spiking activity in M1 and LFP oscillations in DS. We analyzed 1,936 spike-field pairs (121 M1 units and 16 DS LFP channels). To avoid effects of evoked responses on coherence estimates, we subtracted the mean DS event-related potential (ERP) and M1 time-varying firing rate for each cell or LFP channel, respectively, from individual trials before calculating coherence (Figure S2). We saw a profound increase in spike-field coherence across a range of low frequencies in late learning, when rats were skillfully performing the task, relative to early learning (Figure 2C).

We mapped receptive fields in the horizontal dimension by present

We mapped receptive fields in the horizontal dimension by presenting sequences of vertical bars (∼10° wide) having random position (six to nine positions, spanning 56°–77° in azimuth)

and polarity (black or white; Figure 1B). A fraction of the bars (usually 8%) were set to zero contrast to obtain blanks (Figure 1A). Each sequence lasted 20 s, and each bar was flashed for 166 or 200 ms. We generated six such sequences and repeated each five times. We used two types of random sequences: balanced and biased. In balanced sequences, the bars were equally likely to appear at any position (Figures 1A and 1B). In biased sequences, the bars were two to three times more likely to appear at a given position selleck chemical than at any of the other positions (Figures 1C and 1D). The number of blanks was kept the same. We fit each cell with a Linear-Nonlinear-Poisson model (LNP model) that maximized the likelihood of the observed spike trains (Paninski, 2004, Pillow, 2007 and Simoncelli et al., 2004). The nonlinearity was imposed to be the same in the balanced and the biased conditions. In this way, differences in tuning and responsiveness between the balanced and biased conditions are entirely captured by the linear filters. We included a constant offset term so that we could allow for changes in mean activity between the two conditions

(Figures 1E and 1H). We fitted two versions of the LNP model for each cell: one in which the linear filter was convolved with a signed version of the stimulus (as appropriate for linear cells), and one RG7204 clinical trial in which it was convolved with an unsigned version of the Thalidomide stimulus (as appropriate for nonlinear cells). For each cell, we chose the version of the model that gave the highest likelihood of the data. We selected the time slice at which the linear filters were maximal to obtain the spatial tuning curve of each neuron (Figure S1). We fitted these responses with Gaussian functions (Figures 1F, 1G, 1I, and 1J) and used the appropriate parameters to quantify response gain, preferred position, and tuning width

for each neuron. We describe the tuning curve of an LGN neuron as: equation(Equation 1) RLGN(φ,θLGN)=f(φ−θLGN)where φφ is the stimulus position and f()f() is the receptive field profile of an LGN neuron with preferred position θLGNθLGN. We can then construct the response of a V1 neuron with preferred position θV1θV1 to the same stimulus as: equation(Equation 2) RV1(φ,θV1)=(∑θLGNRLGN(φ,θLGN)g(θLGN−θV1))αwhere g()g() is the summation profile of the V1 neuron over LGN. This quantity is integrated over all LGN neurons and passed through a static nonlinearity (αα). Effectively, the V1 neuron weights the population response of LGN by its summation profile. To account for our data, it was sufficient to use simple Gaussian functions to describe both f()f() and g()g().

Testing this central prediction requires the simultaneous activat

Testing this central prediction requires the simultaneous activation of two competing inputs and the simultaneous recording of the rhythm in the group of neurons providing input and

Tyrosine Kinase Inhibitor Library price the rhythm in their target group. To enable a concrete experimental test of CTC, a strong prediction can be derived about the synchronization among local rhythms in monkey areas V1 and V4 during selective attention to one of two simultaneously presented visual stimuli: if two stimuli activate separate sites in V1, and both activate one V4 site equally strongly, then the V4 site should synchronize selectively to the V1 site driven by the attended stimulus. Here, we test this prediction, assessing local rhythms through electrocorticographic (ECoG) local field potential (LFP) recordings. To quantify synchronization between V1 and V4, we used multisite LFP recordings, which have been shown highly effective in assessing long-range, interareal synchronization (Roelfsema Dasatinib manufacturer et al., 1997; von Stein et al., 2000; Tallon-Baudry et al., 2001, 2004). Multisite LFP recordings are routinely carried out with ECoG grid electrodes implanted onto the brains of epilepsy patients for presurgical focus localization. These unique recordings from the human brain have been used for numerous cognitive and/or systems neuroscience studies (Tallon-Baudry

et al., 2001; Canolty et al., 2006), yet they typically do not include early visual areas. We therefore developed a high-density ECoG grid of electrodes (1 mm diameter platinum discs) and implanted it subdurally onto the brains of two macaque monkeys to obtain simultaneous

recordings from 252 electrodes across large parts of the left hemisphere (Rubehn et al., 2009). Figure 1A shows Rolziracetam the brain of monkey P with the electrode positions superimposed (see Figure S1A, which shows electrode positions for both monkeys, available online). Figure 1B illustrates that a contralateral visual stimulus induced strong gamma-band activity (Gray et al., 1989), while an ipsilateral stimulus did not. Figure S1B shows respective time-frequency analyses, demonstrating that stimulus-induced gamma was sustained as long as the stimulus was presented. The gamma band was within the range of frequencies described in previous studies using drifting gratings in human subjects or awake monkeys (Hoogenboom et al., 2006; Fries et al., 2008; Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2009; Swettenham et al., 2009; Vinck et al., 2010; van Pelt et al., 2012). Within that range, the gamma band found here was relatively high, most likely due to the individual predispositions of the animals and the use of moving stimuli (Swettenham et al., 2009) of high contrast (Ray and Maunsell, 2010).

We used an optimal family of orthogonal tapers (slepian functions

We used an optimal family of orthogonal tapers (slepian functions). These are parameterized by their time length T and frequency bandwidth W. For chosen T and W, maximally k = 2TW−1 tapers centered in frequency are appropriate for spectral estimation. Power spectra were

estimated over 0.4 s windows centered SB431542 in vivo on deflections (Figure 5D) and correct trials of 2.5 s (Figure 6B) with time-bandwidth product TW = 2 and k = 3 tapers. The same parameters were used for measuring spike-to-spike coherence during baseline and epochs with the largest number of deflections. To enhance readability of the LFP power at high frequencies, which are masked by the 1/fn power-law decay, we normalized the power by the frequency. We thank E. Antzoulatos, M. Bosch, S. Brincat, T. Buschman, J. Cromer, C. Diogo, M. Moazami, J. Rose, J. Roy, M. Silver, and M. Wicherski for valuable discussions on the manuscript. We also thank B. Gray, K.

MacCully, M. Noble, and D. Ouellette for technical assistance and R. Marini for surgical assistance and veterinary care. This work was supported by CELEST, a National Science Foundation Science of Learning Center (NSF OMA-0835976), NIH-NINDS R01-NS035145, and RG7204 clinical trial the Human Frontiers Science Program Organization (to M.V.P). M.V.P conceived of and designed the experiment. M.V.P performed (and E.K.M supervised) training, electrophysiological recording, and data analysis. M.V.P and E.K.M wrote the paper. “
“Ca2+ GBA3 enters a cell through NMDAR channels only when presynaptic glutamate release and depolarization of the postsynaptic membrane occur simultaneously (correlated activity). Conversely, NMDAR-mediated Ca2+ influx is suppressed at voltages near the resting membrane potential (uncorrelated activity), due to Mg2+ block, a mechanism in which the pore of NMDARs is blocked by external Mg2+ ions (Mayer et al., 1984 and Nowak et al., 1984). Since Mg2+ block allows cells to discriminate between correlated synaptic inputs and uncorrelated activity, NMDARs have been proposed to function as “Hebbian coincidence detectors.” However, the behavioral

significance and molecular effects of Mg2+-block-dependent suppression of Ca2+ influx during uncorrelated activity remains unknown (Single et al., 2000). Functional NMDARs are heteromeric assemblies of an essential NR1 subunit and various NR2 subunits. Studies of NMDAR channels have demonstrated that Mg2+ block is dependent on an asparagine (N) residue at a “Mg2+ block site” located in a putative channel-forming transmembrane segment (TM2, see Figure 2A) of each subunit (Burnashev et al., 1992, Mori et al., 1992 and Single et al., 2000). Drosophila have a single NR2 homolog, dNR2, which contains a glutamine at the Mg2+ block site (Q721), and a single NR1 homolog, dNR1, which contains an N at this site (N631). A previous study has shown that the N631 residue in dNR1 is sufficient for Mg2+ block in flies ( Xia et al., 2005).